Literature Index

Displaying 1821 - 1830 of 3326
  • Author(s):
    Willett, J. B., & Singer, J. D.
    Editors:
    Gordon, F., & Gordon, S.
    Year:
    1992
    Abstract:
    In this paper, we describe how we use real data in the classroom and we identify characteristics of data sets that make them particularly good for teaching. We also identify advantages and disadvantages of this approach, and offer suggestions for overcoming the obstacles. In a separate section of this volume, we provide an annotated bibliography that lists several hundred primary and secondary data sources that teachers may use in their own courses.
  • Author(s):
    Madsen, R.
    Editors:
    Davidson, R. & Swift, J.
    Year:
    1986
    Abstract:
    As teachers, most of us enourage the students in our classes to come to us during special office hours if they wish to receive extra help. Many students feel that they should not "waste" the teachers' time with their questions. Others may feel embarrassed to ask questions in a one-to-one setting. Whatever the reason a student may have, we have felt that students who really need help frequently do not get help through the office-hour channel. We have tried to provide a variety of sources of extra help for students. In this paper we will describe the sources of help and summarize how students utilized these sources over the course of the semester.
    Location:
  • Author(s):
    Ayton, P., Hunt, A. J., & Wright, G.
    Year:
    1989
    Abstract:
    This article presents a critique of the concept of randomness as it occurs in the psychological literature. The first section of our article outlines the significance of a concept of randomness to the process of induction; we need to distinguish random and non-random events in order to perceive lawful regularities and formulate theories concerning events in the world. Next we evaluate the psychological research that has suggested that human concepts of randomness are not normative. We argue that, because the tasks set to experimental subjects are logically problematic, observed biases may be an artifact of the experimental situations and that even if such biases do generalise they may not have pejorative implications for induction in the real world. Thirdly we investigate the statistical methodology utilised in tests for randomness and find it riddled with paradox. In a fourth section we find various branches of scientific endeavour that are stymied by the problems posed by randomness. Finally we briefly mention the social significance of randomness and conclude by arguing that such a fundamental concept merits and requires more serious considerations.
  • Author(s):
    Scholz, R. W.
    Editors:
    Kapadia, R., & Borovcnik, M.
    Year:
    1991
    Abstract:
    This chapter is devoted to the research on probability which may be found in the subject of psychology, and deals with various research paradigms. Salient experimental tasks and research issues on how individuals cope with probabilistic settings are discussed. The objective is to provide a substantiated and representative review of the large number of psychological investigations. We start by presenting an indicative sample of psychological studies on people's response to probabilistic problems. Critical dimensions for judging the educational relevance of paradigms and issues will be introduced. The few developmental theories which deal with the acquisition of probability in psychology will be discussed. Shortcomings and perspectives of the educational research are critically examined in the concluding sections.
  • Author(s):
    Baloglu, M.
    Year:
    2002
    Abstract:
    The Statistics Anxiety Rating Scale has 51 items, each scored on a 5-point rating scale to measure statistics anxiety with six subscales, Worth of Statistics, Interpretation Anxiety, Test and Class Anxiety, Computational Self-concept, Fear of Asking for Help, and Fear of Statistics Teachers. Psychometric properties included analyses of construct and concurrent validities an internal consistency and test-retest reliability. 221 college students (74% women; M age=28 yr.) in elementary statistics courses at several southwestern state universities participated. The findings are consistent with previous reports and indicate adequate concurrent validity, internal consistency, and split-half reliability, but for construct validity confirmatory factor analysis yielded marginal support.
  • Author(s):
    Nguyen, P.
    Editors:
    Goodall, G.
    Year:
    2005
    Abstract:
    Cooking and tasting chicken soup in three different pots of very different size serves to demonstrate that it is the absolute sample size that matters the most in determining the accuracy of the findings of the poll, not the relative sample size, i.e. the size of the sample in relation to its population.
  • Author(s):
    Heaton, R. & Mickelson, W.
    Editors:
    Lee, C. & Satterlee, A.
    Year:
    2003
    Abstract:
    Pfannkuch (1997) contends that variation is a critical issue throughout the statistical inquiry process, from posing a question to drawing conclusions. This is particularly true for K-6 teachers when they attempt to use the process of statistical investigation as a means of teaching and learning across the spectrum of the K-6 curricula. In this context statistical concepts and ideas are taught and learned in conjunction with the important content area ideas and concepts. For a K-6 teacher, this means that the investigation must not only be planned in advance, but also aimed at being responsive to students. The potential for surprise questions, unanticipated responses and unintended outcomes is high, and teachers need to "think on their feet" statistically and react immediately in ways that accomplish content objectives, as well as convey correct statistical principles and reasoning. The intellectual demands in this context are no different than in other instances where teachers are trying to teach for understanding (i.e., Cohen, McLaughlin, &amp; Talbert, 1993; Ma, 1999).<br>In this line of research, we explore the role variability plays in this form of teaching and learning. Simultaneously, we analyze what teachers need to know about variability and be able to do with variability in data so that purposeful investigations into topics of the curriculum can be successful in teaching both statistical concepts and process and the important ideas associated with content. We work from a situative perspective (Greeno, 1997) and analyze the degree to which the statistical knowledge needed for teaching appears to have been learned for understanding (Hiebert &amp; Carpenter, 1992) and leads to generative understanding (Franke, Carpenter, Levi &amp; Fennema, 2001). The findings of this study point toward the situated nature of knowledge about variability needed for and used in teaching and leads to significant implications for the growth of teachers' statistical knowledge.
  • Author(s):
    Mickelson, W. &amp; Heaton, R.
    Editors:
    Lee, C.
    Year:
    2003
    Abstract:
    Pfannkuch (1997) contends that variation is a critical issue throughout the statistical inquiry process, from posing a question to drawing conclusions. This is particularly true for K-6 teachers when they attempt to use the process of statistical investigation as a means of teaching and learning across the spectrum of the K-6 curricula. In this context statistical concepts and ideas are taught and learned in conjunction with the important content area ideas and concepts. For a K-6 teacher, this means that the investigation must not only be planned in advance, but also aimed at being responsive to students. The potential for surprise questions, unanticipated responses and unintended outcomes is high, and teachers need to "think on their feet" statistically and react immediately in ways that accomplish content objectives, as well as convey correct statistical principles and reasoning. The intellectual demands in this context are no different than in other instances where teachers are trying to teach for understanding (i.e., Cohen, McLaughlin, &amp; Talbert, 1993; Ma, 1999).
  • Author(s):
    Kader, G., &amp; Perry, M.
    Year:
    1998
    Abstract:
    This article discusses a game called push-penny that can be used to develop students' intuitive feelings for the consequences of randomness.
  • Author(s):
    Torok, R.
    Year:
    2000
    Abstract:
    This paper examines the role of variation in statistics education and describes a chance and data unit with a focus on variation that has been conducted with three high school mathematics classes.

Pages

The CAUSE Research Group is supported in part by a member initiative grant from the American Statistical Association’s Section on Statistics and Data Science Education

register