Literature Index

Displaying 1771 - 1780 of 3326
  • Author(s):
    Stohl, J.
    Editors:
    Jones, G. A.
    Year:
    2005
    Abstract:
    The success of any probability curriculum for developing students' probabilistic reasoning depends greatly on teachers' understanding of probability as well as a much deeper understanding of issues such as students' misconceptions (Stohl, p. 351, this chapter).<br><br>The purpose of this chapter is to investigate issues concerning the nature and development of teachers' probability understanding. The chapter begins with a discussion of central issues that affect teachers' efforts to facilitate students' probabilistic understanding. I then examine teachers' knowledge and beliefs about probability, their ability to teach probabilistic ideas, and lessons learned from programs in teacher education that have aimed at developing teachers' knowledge about probability.
  • Author(s):
    Stohl, H.
    Editors:
    Jones, G. A.
    Year:
    2005
    Abstract:
    The purpose of this chapter is to investigate issues concerning the nature and development of teachers' probability understanding. An outline of the central issues that affect teachers' efforts to facilitate students' probabilistic understanding is given. Then, I examine teachers' knowledge and beliefs about probability, their ability to teach probabilistic ideas, and lessons learned from programs in teacher education that have aimed at developing teachers' knowledge about probability.
  • Author(s):
    Jones, G. A., Thornton, C. A., &amp; Langrall, C. W.
    Year:
    1997
    Abstract:
    Responding to world wide recommendations that recognize the importance of having younger students develop a greater understanding of probability, this study designed and evaluated a third-grade instructional program in probability. The instructional program was informed by a cognitive framework that describes students' probabilistic thinking and also adopted a socio-constructivist orientation. Two classes participated in the instructional program, one in the fall (early) and the other in the spring (delayed). Following instruction, both groups displayed significant growth in probabilistic thinking that was not simply due to maturation. There was also evidence, based on four target students, that children's readiness to list the outcomes of the sample space, their ability to connect sample space and probability, and their predisposition to use valid number representations in describing probabilities, were key factors in fostering learning.
  • Author(s):
    Kreitler, S., Ziegler, E., &amp; Kreitler, H.
    Year:
    1989
    Abstract:
    This study focused on the relations between performance on a three-choice probability-learning task and conceptions of probability as outlined by Piaget concerning mixture, normal distribution, random selection, odds estimation, and permutations. The probability-learning task and four Piagetian tasks were administered randomly to 100 male and 100 female, middle SES, average IQ children in three age groups (5 to 6, 8 to 9, and 11 to 12 years old) from different schools. Half the children were from Middle Eastern backgrounds, and half were from European or American backgrounds. As predicted, developmental level of probability thinking was related to performance on the probability-learning task. The more advanced the child's probability thinking, the higher his or her level of maximization and hypothesis formulation and testing and the lower his or her level of systematically patterned responses. The results suggest that the probability-learning and Piagetian tasks assess similar cognitive skills and that performance on the probability-learning task reflects a variety of probability concepts.
  • Author(s):
    Maxine Pfannkuch, Stephanie Budgett, Rachel Fewster, Marie Fitch, Simeon Pattenwise, Chris Wild, and Ilze Ziedins
    Year:
    2016
    Abstract:
    Because new learning technologies are enabling students to build and explore probability models, we believe that there is a need to determine the big enduring ideas that underpin probabilistic thinking and modeling. By uncovering the elements of the thinking modes of expert users of probability models we aim to provide a base for the setting of new and more relevant goals for probability education in the 21st century. We interviewed seven practitioners, whose professional lives are centered on probability modeling over a diverse range of fields including the development of probability theory. A thematic analysis approach produced four frameworks: (1) probability modeling approaches; (2) probabilistic thinking approaches to a problem; (3) a probability modeling cycle; and (4) core building blocks for probabilistic thinking and modeling. The main finding was that seeing structure and applying structure were important aspects of probability modeling. The implications of our findings for probability education are discussed.
  • Author(s):
    Schwertman, N. C., McCready, T. A., &amp; Howard, L.
    Year:
    1991
    Abstract:
    The authors state that athletics in general can be used to demonstrate probabilistic concepts and that these may be reinforced due to the motivational aspects of the examples. In this article, the planification of the NCAA Regional Basketball tournaments is examined as a probability problem (e.g. what is the probability of team #3 winning the regionals?). In calculating the probability of any team winning, the students must: 1) evaluate the probability of each game played; 2) assume independence of each contest; and 3) incorporate the relative strength of each team in the model. Three probability models are considered, the third being the most adequate. It seems to provide a good fit to the data and is considered interesting to students without requiring a strong mathematical background. The key point in this article is that these models demonstrate the multiplication principle and the additive property of mutually exclusive events in addition to the motivation provided by the topic itself.
  • Author(s):
    Green, D. R.
    Year:
    1990
    Abstract:
    This report presents results of tests undertaken with English school pupils aged from 7 to 14 years during 1986 and 1990. This first edition of the report concentrates on presenting basic statistical analyses of the various tests carried out. A future edition will include more detailed results for those questions concerning spatial distribution which are not yet fully analysed, together with more sophisticated analyses.
  • Author(s):
    Pfannkuch, M., Seber, G. A. F. &amp; Wild, C. J.
    Editors:
    Goodall, G.
    Year:
    2002
    Abstract:
    The teaching of probability theory has been steadily declining in introductory statistics courses as students have difficulty with handling the rules of probability. In this article, we give a data-driven approach, based on two-way tables, which helps students to become familiar with using the usual rules but without the formal structure.
  • Author(s):
    Jennings B. Marshall
    Year:
    2007
    Abstract:
    This article describes how roulette can be used to teach basic concepts of probability. Various bets are used to illustrate the computation of expected value. A betting system shows variations in patterns that often appear in random events.
  • Author(s):
    Buckheister, P. G.
    Year:
    1994
    Abstract:
    The Curriculum and Evaluation Standards for School Mathematics (NCTM 1989) states in Standard 11 for grades 9 through 12 that students should have oportunities to "use experimental and theoretical probabilities to represent and solve problems involving ncertainty." Standard 1 emphasizes the importance of students' learning to "formulate problems from situations within and outside mathematics." This article discusses a simply stated problem involving uncertainty that students can investigate experimentally or theoretcially. The problem places students in the role of problem formulator by giving them opportunities to generate various interesting problems of their own on the basis of a given situation. By changing certain characteristics of the original problem, students can be introduced to some fundamental concepts of decision making in two -player games.
    Location:

Pages

The CAUSE Research Group is supported in part by a member initiative grant from the American Statistical Association’s Section on Statistics and Data Science Education