Literature Index

Displaying 891 - 900 of 3326
  • Author(s):
    Alldredge, J. R., Johnson, H. D., & Sanchez, J. J.
    Editors:
    Rossman, A., & Chance, B.
    Year:
    2006
    Abstract:
    Recent studies have indicated that student attitudes can assist or impede learning and that students tend to have negative attitudes towards mathematics and statistics. We used a treatment-control experimental design to explore the effects of using video clips, showing applications of statistics in real world settings, on student attitudes toward statistics. Students with higher scores on standardized tests of mathematical and verbal ability, who were exposed to the video treatment, had more positive attitudes toward statistics than video exposed students with lower ability scores and more than students who were not exposed to the video treatment.
  • Author(s):
    Amy G. Froelich, William M. Duckworth, and Jessica Culhane
    Year:
    2009
    Abstract:
    Since the introduction of the first iPod portable music player (MP3 player) by Apple, Inc., users have questioned the randomness of the shuffle feature. Most evidence cited by users claiming to show nonrandom behavior in the shuffle feature is anecdotal in nature and not based on any systematic analysis of its randomness. This article reports on our attempt to investigate the shuffle feature on the iPod and to test its randomness through the use of probability and statistical modeling. We begin by reviewing the research on people's inability to perceive and understand both random and nonrandom behavior. Probability models are then developed, under the assumption of a random shuffle, for several of the most common types of events cited as evidence of a nonrandom shuffle. Under this null hypothesis of a random shuffle, several goodness-of-fit tests of one of the probability models are conducted using data collected from real iPods. No evidence to support user claims of a nonrandom shuffle was found. Finally, we conclude with some reflections on and ideas for incorporating these examples into undergraduate probability and statistics courses.
  • Author(s):
    Watson, J. M.
    Editors:
    Phillips, B.
    Year:
    2002
    Abstract:
    As teachers of statistics we know the fundamental components of statistical enquiry, be it classical or exploratory. When we turn the focus on ourselves as statistics educators, we run the risk of forgetting some of the fundamental principles of good research - principles that are broader than carrying out statistical significance tests. In this talk I want to present some examples of research in statistics education to illustrate the stages and outcomes that contribute to results that have a scholarly impact on the statistics education community. As a single teacher with a good idea on how to teach "confidence intervals," I do not expect anyone to pay much attention to me. If I can, however, place my ideas in the context of others' ideas or research on teaching confidence intervals; conduct a study - maybe a case study or a controlled experimental<br>design - that is valid for considering the issue I want to promote in teaching about confidence intervals; and have my results refereed by peers in the field; then I can expect people to pay attention to me.
  • Author(s):
    Lopes, L. L.
    Editors:
    Arkes, H. R., &amp; Hammond, K. R.
    Year:
    1986
    Abstract:
    In this article I discuss the fundamental relation between people's ability to do induction and their beliefs about randomness or noise, and I illustrate the special difficulties that psychologists face when they try to evaluate the rationality of these beliefs. The presentation is divided into four sections. The first describes the traditional experimental approach to evaluating people's conceptions of randomness and summarizes the data that have been taken to support the conclusion that people have a very poor conception of randomness. The second contrasts the relatively narrow conception of randomness that one finds in philosophical and mathematical treatments of the topic. The third outlines some benefits for psychologists to be gained from thinking about induction as a problem in signal detection., and the fourth presents the argument that any adequate evaluation of ordinary people's conceptions of randomness must consider the role that these conceptions play an inductive inference, that is, in distinguishing between random and nonrandom events. Originally appeared in the Journal of Experimental Psychology; Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 1982, 8 (6), 626 - 636
  • Author(s):
    Caro-Ann Badcock
    Year:
    2008
    Abstract:
    The development of safe, efficacious and cost effective medicines and medical devices requires<br>dedicated teams of people with diverse backgrounds including all manner of the sciences,<br>regulatory affairs, marketing, health economics and so on. Statistics and statisticians also have<br>an integral role in the drug development industry. In fact, it is probably one of the few industries<br>where there is a regulatory requirement that statisticians MUST be involved in each project from<br>the design of an experiment through to the report. The life cycle of a medicinal product, focusing<br>on the statistical needs of various team members and others exposed to the product will be<br>described to understand the range of statistical skills required by the various disciplines within<br>the pharmaceutical, medical device and biotechnology sectors.
  • Author(s):
    Pablo Carranza and Alain Kuzniak
    Year:
    2008
    Abstract:
    In epistemological studies, two main interpretations of probabilities are the frequentist and the Bayesian. In this paper we first show that both interpretations are present in French Secondary schools, albeit the official curriculum only supports the frequentist approach. We then suggest a possible teaching situation to introduce teachers and students to the subjective-objective duality with some statistics training situations via the use of problem solving. We also give some characteristics and conditions useful to build situations fitted to this goal.
  • Author(s):
    Seierr, E. &amp; Robe, C.
    Editors:
    Goodall, G.
    Year:
    2002
    Abstract:
    Testing statistical hypotheses introduces new vocabulary, concepts and a way of thinking that some students might initially find difficult. We provide a simple case that can be used in class as a gentle introduction to the ideas and procedures of hypothesis testing.
  • Author(s):
    Makar, K., Confrey, J.
    Abstract:
    Bakker (2002) identified two categories of learning software in mathematics: landscape-type software and route-type software. Route type software was designed to guide learners through a hypothetical learning trajectory with a fairly fixed destination. Alternatively, landscape-type software is designed to support learners in conducting open-ended investigations. The use of these dynamic software tools for the learning of mathematics and statistics has gained increasing prominence in schools because of its ability to support multiple purposes defined by the user rather than the software. Little is known, however, about the diversity of approaches in which learners use these software packages to conduct investigations. This paper reports on a study of eighteen prospective secondary mathematics and science teachers' approaches to conducting a statistical investigation using the hynamic data analysis software FathomTM (Finzer, 2001). Three distinct approaches were identified by the research-- Wonderers, Wanderers, and Answerers--each with measurable and quantitative differences in these approaches as well as their potential epistemological roots.
  • Author(s):
    Sandra R. Madden
    Year:
    2008
    Abstract:
    Fifty-six high school mathematics teachers participated in a four-day technology-intensive professional development experience designed to support their understanding of the big statistical idea of "comparing distributions." Content pretests and teacher interviews informed the hypothetical learning trajectory and design of professional development beyond that from the research literature. Additional data sources included content post-tests and interviews, video-tape of the professional development experience, and teachers' constructed responses to written reflection prompts during the session. Retrospective analyses surfaced a striking phenomenon I have chosen to call dynamic technology scaffolding (DTS) which involves coordination of increasingly-sophisticated technological tools during statistical investigation with the purpose of supporting learners' conceptual understanding of an important statistical big idea.
  • Author(s):
    Weissglass, J., &amp; Cummings, D.
    Editors:
    W. Zimmerman &amp; S. Cunningham
    Year:
    1991
    Abstract:
    This paper will describe one approach to using visualization in learning statistics. The approach uses the ability of computers to perform statistical experiments (with the parameters determined by the learner) and display the results dynamically (as they occur). We will refer to the method as dynamic visual experimentation. We will use the term random phenomena to refer to the broad class of situations whose mathematical analysis requires statistical or probabilistic concepts or methods.

Pages

The CAUSE Research Group is supported in part by a member initiative grant from the American Statistical Association’s Section on Statistics and Data Science Education

register