Literature Index

Displaying 2701 - 2710 of 3326
  • Author(s):
    Rosemary Callingham and Jane M. Watson
    Year:
    2017
    Abstract:
    Statistical literacy increasingly is considered an important outcome of schooling. There is little information, however, about appropriate expectations of students at different stages of schooling. Some progress towards this goal was made by Watson and Callingham (2005), who identified an empirical 6-level hierarchy of statistical literacy and the distribution of middle school students across the levels, using archived data from 1993-2000. There is interest in reconsidering these outcomes a decade later, during which statistics and probability has become a recognised strand of the Australian mathematics curriculum. Using a new data-set of over 7000 student responses from middle-years students in different parts of Australia during the period 2007-2009, the nature of the hierarchy was confirmed. Longitudinal analysis identified how students performed across time against the hierarchy. Suggestions are made for systems and teachers about realistic expectations for middle-years students, and possible curriculum challenges.
  • Author(s):
    Terán, T. E.
    Editors:
    Phillips, B.
    Year:
    2002
    Abstract:
    When the Federal Law of Education in Argentine (1994) changed the curriculum of schools, statistics contents were introduced in the initial level (from 3 to 5 years), in the E.G.B. (from 6 to 15 years) and in the Polimodal (from 15 to 18 years). It was necessary to prepare teachers in statistics as they never have this signature in their curriculum. In this paper I will show how all this process is taking place in Argentine. In the universities, it depends on the careers, but with the curriculum changes, the purpose is to introduce courses of statistics not only on the grade careers, but also in the masters courses.
  • Author(s):
    Gelman, A., & Stern, H.
    Year:
    2006
    Abstract:
    It is common to summarize statistical comparisons by declarations of statistical<br>significance or non-significance. Here we discuss one problem with such declarations,<br>namely that changes in statistical significance are often not themselves statistically<br>significant. By this, we are not merely making the commonplace observation that<br>any particular threshold is arbitrary - for example, only a small change is required to<br>move an estimate from a 5.1% significance level to 4.9%, thus moving it into statistical<br>significance. Rather, we are pointing out that even large changes in significance levels<br>can correspond to small, non-significant changes in the underlying variables.<br>The error we describe is conceptually different from other oft-cited problems - that<br>statistical significance is not the same as practical importance, that dichotomization into<br>significant and non-significant results encourages the dismissal of observed differences<br>in favor of the usually less interesting null hypothesis of no difference, and that any<br>particular threshold for declaring significance is arbitrary. We are troubled by all of<br>these concerns and do not intend to minimize their importance. Rather, our goal is to<br>bring attention to what we have found is an important but much less discussed point.<br>We illustrate with a theoretical example and two applied examples.
  • Author(s):
    Garfield, J., & Ben-Zvi, D.
    Editors:
    C. Batanero
    Year:
    2007
  • Author(s):
    Sedlmeier, P.
    Year:
    1998
    Abstract:
    Different studies on how well people take sample size into account have found a wide range of solution rates. In a recent review Sedlmeier and Gigerenzer (1997) suggested that a substantial part of the variation in results can be explained by the fact that experimenters have used two different types of sample-size tasks, one involving frequency distributions and the other sampling distributions. This suggestion rested on an analysis of studies that , with one exception, did not systematically manipulate type of distribution versions. In Study 1, a substantial difference between solution rates for the two types of tasks was found. Study 2 replicated this finding and ruled out an alternative explanation for it, namely, that the solution rate for sampling distribution tasks was lower because the information they contained was harder to extract than that in frequency distribution tasks. Finally, in Study 3 an attempt was make to reduce the gap between the solution rates for the two types of tasks by giving participants as many hints, the gap in performance remained. A new computational model of statistical reasoning specifies cognitive processes that might explain why people are better at solving frequency than sampling distribution tasks.
  • Author(s):
    Bakker, A.
    Year:
    2003
    Abstract:
    The Journal of Statistics Education (JSE) has a unique structure and an inclusive philosophy that have technical consequences for readers and authors. This paper, a message from the journal's managing editor, explains why the JSE was built to have its unique structure, the format of information available to readers, and the effect the philosophy will have. The paper's Appendix outlines three groups of readers and associated methods of accessing the journal. The Appendix also describes the purpose and contents of the associated JSE Information Service.
  • Author(s):
    Cohen, J.
    Year:
    1994
    Abstract:
    After 4 decades of severe criticism, the ritual of null hypothesis significance testing - mechanical dichotomous decisions around a sacred .05 criterion - still persists. This article reviews the problems with this practice, including its near-universal misinterpretation of p as the probability that H 0 is false, the misinterpretation that its complement is the probability of successful replication, and the mistaken assumption that if one rejects H 0 one thereby affirms the theory that led to the test. Exploratory data analysis and the use of graphic methods, a steady improvement in and a movement toward standardization in measurement, an emphasis on estimating effect sizes using confidence intervals, and the informed use of available statistical methods is suggested. For generalization, psychologists must finally rely, as has been done in all the older sciences, on replication.
  • Author(s):
    Kapadia, R., &amp; Borovcnik, M.
    Editors:
    Kapadia, R., &amp; Borovcnik, M.
    Year:
    1991
    Abstract:
    This opening chapter presents the aims and rationale of the book within an appropriate theoretical framework. Initially, we provide the reader with an orientation of what the book intends to achieve. The next section highlights some important issues in mathematical education, establishing a framework against which ideas in the book have been developed. Partly, the research has been inspired by the first series on mathematical education: Freudenthal's Didactical Phenomenology of Mathematical Structures. Though he considers many topics in mathematics he excludes (perhaps surprisingly) probability. Finally, summaries of each of the chapters are related to these didactic approaches.
  • Author(s):
    Giambalvo, O., Milito, A. M. &amp; Marsala, M. R.
    Editors:
    Phillips, B.
    Year:
    2002
    Abstract:
    The present work aims to investigate the educational value of statistics as perceived by teachers involved in the teaching of this subject in schools of every type and level in the city of Palermo. To achieve this aim a preliminary fact-finding investigation was performed.
  • Author(s):
    Lisa J. Carnell
    Year:
    2008
    Abstract:
    Students often enter an introductory statistics class with less than positive attitudes about the subject.<br>They tend to believe statistics is difficult and irrelevant to their lives. Observational evidence from<br>previous studies suggests including projects in a statistics course may enhance students' attitudes toward<br>statistics. This study examines the relationship between inclusion of a student-designed data collection<br>project in an introductory statistics course and 6 components comprising students' attitudes toward<br>statistics. The sample consisted of 42 college students enrolled in an introductory statistics course.<br>Comparisons of those who completed the student-designed data collection project (n = 24) and those who<br>did not complete the project (n = 18) suggest that inclusion of a project may not significantly impact<br>students' attitudes toward statistics. However, these findings must be viewed as only a preliminary step in<br>the study of the effect of projects on attitudes toward statistics.

Pages

The CAUSE Research Group is supported in part by a member initiative grant from the American Statistical Association’s Section on Statistics and Data Science Education

register