Literature Index

Displaying 2021 - 2030 of 3326
  • Author(s):
    Wainer, H., Robinson, D. H.
    Year:
    2003
    Abstract:
    Recent criticisms of null hypothesis significance testing (NHST) have appeared in education and psychology research journals (e.g., Cohen, 1990, 1994; Kupfersmid, 1988; Rosenthal, 1991; Rosnow & Rosenthal, 1989; Shaver, 1985; Sohn, 2000; Thompson, 1994, 1997; see also Research in the Schools [1998]). In this article we discuss these criticisms for both current use of NHST and plausible future use. We suggest that the historical use of such procedures was reasonable and that current users might spend time profitably reading some of Fisher's applied work. However, we also believe that modifications to NHST and to the interpretations of its outcomes might better suit the needs of modern science. Our primary conclusion is that NHST is most often useful as an adjunct to other results (e.g., effect sizes) rather than as a stand-alone result. We cite some examples, however, where NHST can be profitably used alone. Last, we find considerable experimental support for a less rigid attitude toward the interpretation of the probability yielded from such procedures.
  • Author(s):
    Carvalho, C. & César, M.
    Editors:
    Phillips, B.
    Year:
    2002
    Abstract:
    The role of Statistics is becoming increasingly important in today's society. According to several authors, collaborative work has shown to be one of the most adapted forms of facilitating knowledge appropriation and the mobilisation of competencies. The project Interaction and Knowledge has studied and encouraged peer interactions in the Mathematics class, in association with a new didactic or experimental contract, as a way of promoting pupils' performances, allowing them to reach relational knowledge. By analysing excerpts of these interactions we can understand the facilitating character of this working method.
  • Author(s):
    Kaur, B.
    Editors:
    Vere-Jones, D., Carlyle, S., & Dawkins, B. P.
    Year:
    1991
    Abstract:
    Statistics is taught in all Singapore schools as part of the compulsory subject mathematics. Only at the university level may statistics be offered as a subject by itself, in the Department of Economics and Statistics. In attempting to make every school-leaver numerate and literate the statistics taught at the school level is meant to equip an individual with the ability to interpret and understand correctly the data presented in tables, diagrams, charts, and graphs. It is the concern of every mathematics teacher in school that pupils should know enough about simple statistics to be able to interpret them correctly and not be deceived by them.
  • Author(s):
    Terry Goodman
    Year:
    2010
    Abstract:
    A probability exploration built on shooting basketball free throws leads unexpectedly to the golden ratio.
  • Author(s):
    Ross, L., & Anderson, C. A.
    Editors:
    Kahneman, D., Slovic, P., & Tversky, A.
    Year:
    1982
    Abstract:
    Attribution theory, in its broadest sense, is concerned with the attempts of ordinary people to understand the causes and implications of the events they witness. It deals with the "naive psychology" of people as they interpret their own behavior and the actions of others. The current ascendancy of attribution theory in social psychology thus culminates a long struggle to upgrade that discipline's conception of man. No longer the stimulus-response (S-R) automation of radical behaviorism, promoted beyond the rank of information processor and cognitive consistency seeker, psychological man has at last been awarded a status equal to that of the scientist who investigates him. For in the perspective of attribution theory, people are intuitive psychologists who seek to explain behavior and to draw inferences about actors and about their social environments. The intuitive scientist's ability to master his social environment, accordingly, will depend upon the accuracy and adequacy of his hypotheses, evidence, and analyses. Conversely, any systematic errors in existing theories, biases in available data, or inadequacies in methods of analysis, yield serious consequences - both for the lay psychologist and for the society that he builds and perpetuates. These shortcomings, explored from the vantage point of contemporary attribution theory, provide the focus of this chapter.
  • Author(s):
    Moore, D. S.
    Year:
    1988
    Abstract:
    No! Statistics is no more a branch of mathematics than is economics, and should no more be taught by mathematicians. It is a separate discipline that makes heavy and essential use of mathematical tools, but has origins, subject matter, foundational questions and standards that are distinct from those of mathematics. It is true that many advanced texts and research papers in statistics use formidable mathematics, but this is misleading. After all, many a graduate microeconomics text cites the Kuhn-Tucker theorem on the first page, and many research papers in physics are intensely mathematical. Statistics is as much a distinct discipline as are economics and physics. Its subject matter is data and inference from data. It is unprofessional for mathematicians who lack training and experience in working with data to teach statistics.
  • Author(s):
    Fidler, F.
    Editors:
    Rossman, A., & Chance, B.
    Year:
    2006
    Abstract:
    Several editorial and institutional interventions in psychology have aimed to improve statistical reporting in journals. These efforts have sought to de-emphasise statistical significance and encourage alternative analyses, especially effect sizes and confidence intervals (CIs), but the interventions to date have had short-lived and superficial impact-if any impact at all. I review some of these interventions in psychology and discuss possible reasons for lack of success. I give an inter-disciplinary context by discussing reform efforts in medicine-in which useful reform has already been achieved-and ecology. I then identify statistics education as the next major challenge for reformers, and report data on students' understanding of CIs, and difficulties they have making appropriate interpretation of CIs. I explain the need for further evidence on which to base improved statistics education in psychology.
  • Author(s):
    Bakker, A., Biehler, R., Konold, C.
    Editors:
    Burrill, G.
    Year:
    2005
    Abstract:
    In this chapter, we explore the challenges of learning about box plots and question the rationalefor introducing box plots to middle school students (up to 14 years old). Box plots are very valuable tools for data analysis and for those who know how to interpret them. Research has shown, however, that some of their features make them particularly difficult for young students to use in authentic contexts. These difficulties include: a) box plots generally do not allow perceiving individual cases; b) box plots operate differently than other displays students encounter; c) the median is not as intuitive to students as we once suspected; d) quartiles divide the data into groups in ways that few students (or even teachers) really understand.We recommend that educators consider these features as they determine whether, how, and when to introduce box plots to students.
  • Author(s):
    Falk, R., & Greenbaum, C. W.
    Year:
    1993
    Abstract:
    We present a critique showing the flawed logical structure of statistical significance tests. We then attempt to analyze, why, in spite of this faulty reasoning the use of significance tests persists. We identify the illusion of probabilistic proof by contradiction as a central stumbling block, because it is based on a misleading generalization of reasoning from logic to inference under uncertainty. We present new data from a student sample and examples from the psychological literature showing the strength and prevalence of this illusion. We identify some intrinsic cognitive mechanisms (similarity to modus tollens reasoning; verbal ambiguity in describing the meaning of significance tests; and the need to rule out chance findings) and extrinsic social pressures which help to maintain the illusion. We conclude by mentioning some alternative methods for presenting and analyzing psychological data, none of which can be considered the ultimate method.
  • Author(s):
    Kelly, A. E., Sloane, F., & Whittaker, A.
    Editors:
    Gal, I., & Garfield, J. B.
    Year:
    1997
    Abstract:
    Statistics should be introduced with clear linkages to the mathematics that students already understand and within contexts that students find meaningful. Otherwise, students may learn statistics in rote fashion or apply statistics in a merely instrumental fashion and draw erroneous conclusions from data. In this chapter we present two examples of the use of simple assessment techniques that uncovered students' poor understanding of statistical concepts.

Pages

The CAUSE Research Group is supported in part by a member initiative grant from the American Statistical Association’s Section on Statistics and Data Science Education

register