Literature Index

Displaying 1871 - 1880 of 3326
  • Author(s):
    Ben-Zvi, D.
    Year:
    2004
    Abstract:
    Variability stands in the heart of statistics theory and practice. Concepts and judgments involved in comparing groups have been found to be a productive vehicle for motivating learners to reason statistically and are critical for building the intuitive foundation for inferential reasoning. The focus in this paper is on the emergence of beginners’ reasoning about variation in a comparing distributions situation during their extended encounters with an Exploratory Data Analysis (EDA) curriculum in a technological environment. The current case study is offered as a contribution to understanding the process of constructing meanings and appreciation for variability within a distribution and between distributions and the mechanisms involved therein. It concentrates on the detailed qualitative analysis of the ways by which two seventh grade students started to develop views (and tools to support them) of variability in comparing groups using various statistical representations. Learning statistics is conceived as cognitive development and socialization processes into the culture and values of “doing statistics” (enculturation). In the light of the analysis, a description of what it may mean to begin reasoning about variability in comparing distributions of equal size is proposed, and implications are drawn.  
  • Author(s):
    Reading, C. & Shaughnessy, J. M.
    Editors:
    Ben-Zvi, D. & Garfield, J.
    Year:
    2004
    Abstract:
    "Variation is the reason why people have had to develop sophisticated statistical methods to filter out any messages in data from the surrounding noise" (Wild &amp; Pfannkuch, 1999, p. 236). Both variation, as a concept, and reasoning, as a process, are central to the study of statistics and as such warrant attention from both researchers and educators. This discussion of some recent research attempts to highlight the importance of reasoning about variation. Evolving models of cognitive development in statistical reasoning have been discussed earlier in this book (Chapter 5). The focus in this chapter is on some specific aspects of reasoning about variation.<br>After discussing the nature of variation and its role in the study of statistics, we will introduce some relevant aspects of statistics education. The purpose of the chapter is twofold: first, a review of recent literature concerned, directly or indirectly, with variation; and second, the details of one recent study that investigates reasoning about variation in a sampling situation for students aged 9 to 18. In conclusion, implications from this research for both curriculum development and teaching practice are outlined.
  • Author(s):
    Cl&aacute;udia Borim da Silva and Cileda de Queiroz e Silva Coutinho
    Year:
    2008
    Abstract:
    Variation is a fundamental concept in statistics literacy; standard deviation is part of compulsory school curriculum in Brazil. The objective of this study is to explore reasoning about variability by teachers, using the model proposed by Garfield (2002). The sample was composed of nine in-service mathematics teachers who took part in a teacher-training course on statistics. An experimental focus made it possible for them to experience all the steps of a statistics research project in which the course content was designed to expose the reasoning about variability employed by these teachers. We identified an oscillation between idiosyncratic and procedural levels, but no teacher showed complete reasoning about variation. The most prevalent reasoning employed was verbal, when teachers interpreted standard deviation as a measure of variation among observations.
  • Author(s):
    Chris Reading and Jackie Reid
    Year:
    2007
    Abstract:
    This paper reports one recent study that was part of a project investigating tertiary students' understanding of variation. These students completed a questionnaire prior to, and at the end of, an introductory statistics course and this paper focuses on interviews of selected students designed to determine whether more information could have been gathered about the students' reasoning. Clarification during interviews reinforced researcher interpretation of responses. Prompting assisted students to develop better quality responses but probing was mostly useful for assisting students to re-express reasoning already presented. Cognitive conflict situations proved challenging. The diversity of activities identified by students as assisting the development of their understanding provides a challenge for educators in planning teaching sequences. Both educators and researchers need to listen to students to better understand the development of reasoning.
  • Author(s):
    Carol S. Parke
    Year:
    2008
    Abstract:
    Although graduate students in education are frequently required to write papers throughout their<br>coursework, they typically have limited experience in communicating in the language of statistics, both<br>verbally and in written form. To succeed in their future careers, students must be provided with<br>opportunities to develop deep understandings of concepts, develop reasoning skills, and become familiar<br>with verbalizing and writing about statistics. The instructional approach described here spans the entire<br>semester of a statistics course and consists of several aspects including cognitively rich individual<br>assignments, small group activities, and a student-led scoring activity. To demonstrate the impact of this<br>approach on student learning, qualitative and quantitative data were collected from students in two<br>statistics courses. Several assessments indicate improvement in students' reasoning and understanding,<br>written and verbal communication, and confidence.
  • Author(s):
    Hammerman, J. K., &amp; Rubin, A.
    Year:
    2003
    Abstract:
    Teachers often come to our professional development programs thinking that statistics<br>is about mean, median, and mode. They know how to calculate these statistics, though<br>they don't often have robust images about what they mean or how they're used. When<br>they begin working with computer data analysis tools to explore real data sets, they are<br>able to see and manipulate the data in a way that hasn't really been possible for them<br>before. They identify particular points on graphs and can interpret what their positions<br>mean. They notice clumps and gaps in the data and generally find that there's a lot<br>more to see in the data than they ever imagined before. In addition, those exploring<br>data in this way often ground their interpretations in the range of things they know<br>from the contexts surrounding the data. They discover the richness and complexity of<br>data.<br>Yet all this detail and complexity can be overwhelming. Without some method of<br>focusing or organizing or otherwise reducing the complexity, it can be impossible to say<br>anything at all about a data set. How do people decide what aspects of a data set are<br>important to attend to? What methods do they use to reduce the cognitive load of trying<br>to attend to all the points? This paper will begin to describe some of the ways that<br>people do this while using new, software-driven representational tools. In the process,<br>we will begin to sketch out a framework for the techniques that people develop as they<br>reason in the presence of variability.
  • Author(s):
    Rosebery, A. S., &amp; Rubin, A.
    Year:
    1989
    Abstract:
    Many high school and college statistics courses, however, do not teach statistical reasoning effectively. Rather than helping students understand how to interpret the statistical statements they encounter, these courses focus upon statistical formulas and tests. We believe that the conventional approach not only leaves students confused about fundamental statistical concepts, but also makes the mathematics involved in statistics more rather than less obscure.
  • Author(s):
    Nicholson, J., Ridgway, J. &amp; McCusker, S.
    Editors:
    Goodall, G.
    Year:
    2006
    Abstract:
    Reasoning with data is already pervasive in society, and its importance as a life skill is increasing. We argue that the current statistics curriculum in the United Kingdom at the secondary level does not prepare our young people adequately, and suggest ways in which it could be improved.
  • Author(s):
    Ridgway, J, Nicholson, J., &amp; McCusker, S.
    Editors:
    Rossman, A., &amp; Chance, B.
    Year:
    2006
    Abstract:
    Computers facilitate reasoning with complex data. We report a study where 195 students aged 12 to 15 years were presented with computer based tasks that require reasoning with multivariate data, together with paper based tasks from a well established scale of statistical literacy. All the tasks fitted well onto a single Rasch scale; computer tasks were cognitively more complex, but ranked only slightly more difficult than paper tasks on the Rasch scale. Implications for assessment, the curriculum, and public presentations of data are discussed.
  • Author(s):
    Jim Ridgway, James Nicholson and Sean McCusker
    Editors:
    Carmen Batanero
    Year:
    2007
    Abstract:
    We report a study where 195 students aged 12 to 15 years were presented with computerbased<br>tasks that require reasoning with multivariate data, together with paper-based tasks from a well<br>established scale of statistical literacy. The computer tasks were cognitively more complex, but were only<br>slightly more difficult than paper tasks. All the tasks fitted well onto a single Rasch scale. Implications for<br>the curriculum, and public presentations of data are discussed.

Pages

The CAUSE Research Group is supported in part by a member initiative grant from the American Statistical Association’s Section on Statistics and Data Science Education