Literature Index

Displaying 1051 - 1060 of 3326
  • Author(s):
    Winne, P. H., Gupta, L., & Nesbit, J. C.
    Year:
    1994
    Abstract:
    Describes elementary graph theory statistics, features that characterize differences in cognitive study strategies, and the use of STUDY for gathering trace data for research on patterns of cognition. STUDY affords a high degree of learner control and is an excellent medium for collecting data on individual differences. STUDY users navigate through content and apply studying actions such as underlining, taking notes, requesting elaborations, and attempting test items. As this happens, STUDY creates detailed time-stamped records of the learner's interactions in a log file. This sequence of study actions is reduced to a set of nodes representing action types and a set of links representing a temporal relation. The output of STUDY can yield resemblance statistics that can allow comparison of single actions by a student as well as comparison across students to reveal differences in cognitive processing routines. (French abstract) (PsycLIT Database Copyright 1995 American Psychological Assn, all rights reserved)
  • Author(s):
    Rubin, A., Hammerman, J. K. L., & Konold, C.
    Editors:
    Rossman, A., & Chance, B.
    Year:
    2006
    Abstract:
    Most statistics educators would agree that statistical inference is both the central objective of statistical reasoning and one of the most difficult ideas for students to understand. In traditional approaches, statistical inference is introduced as a quantitative problem, usually of figuring out the probability of obtaining an observed result on the assumption that the null hypothesis is true. In this article, we lay out an alternative approach towards teaching statistical inference that we are calling "informal inference." We begin by describing informal inference and then illustrate ways we have been trying to develop the component ideas of informal inference in a recent data analysis seminar with teachers; our particular emphasis in this article is on the ways in which teachers used TinkerPlots, a statistical visualization tool. After describing teachers' approaches to an inferential task, we offer some preliminary hypotheses about the conceptual issues that arose for them.
  • Author(s):
    Lesser, L. M.
    Year:
    1997
    Abstract:
    Spreadsheets are used to explore the lottery, addressing common misconceptions about various lottery "strategies" and probabilities and providing real-world applications of topics such as discrete probability distributions, combinatorics, sampling, simulation and expected value. Additional pedagogical issues are also discussed. Examples discussed include the probability that an integer appearing in consecutive drawings, the probability that a single 6-ball drawing includes at least two consecutive integers, the probability that exactly one person wins the jackpot, and the probability that a frequent player eventually wins the jackpot.
  • Author(s):
    Sashi Sharma
    Year:
    2007
    Abstract:
    Concerns about the importance of variation in statistics education and a<br>lack of research in this topic led to a preliminary study which explored<br>pre-service teachers' ideas in this area. The teachers completed a written<br>questionnaire about variation in sampling and distribution contexts.<br>Responses were categorised in relation to a framework that identified levels<br>of statistical thinking. The results suggest that while many of the students<br>appeared to acknowledge variation, they were not able to provide adequate<br>explanations. Although the pre-service teachers have had more real-life experiences<br>involving statistics and have been involved in the study of statistical<br>concepts at secondary school level, they still demonstrated the same misconceptions<br>as those of younger students reported in research literature. While<br>more students showed competence on the sampling question, they were less<br>competent on the distribution task. This could be due to task format or<br>contextual issues. The paper concludes by suggesting some implications for<br>further research and teaching.
  • Author(s):
    Heinz, G., Peterson, L. J., Johnson, R. W., &amp; Kerk, C. J.
    Year:
    2003
    Abstract:
    Body girth measurements and skeletal diameter measurements, as well as age, weight, height and gender, are given for 507 physically active individuals - 247 men and 260 women. These data can be used to provide statistics students practice in the art of data analysis. Such analyses range from simple descriptive displays to more complicated multivariate analyses such as multiple regression and discriminant analysis.
  • Author(s):
    delMas, R. &amp; Liu, Y.
    Year:
    2005
    Abstract:
    This study investigated introductory statistics students' conceptual understanding of the standard deviation. A computer environment was designed to promote students' ability to coordinate characteristics of variation of values about the mean with the size of the standard deviation as a measure of that variation. Twelve students participated in an interview divided into two primary phases, an exploration phase where students rearranged histogram bars to produce the largest and smallest standard deviation, and a testing phase where students compared the sizes of the standard deviation of two distributions. Analysis of data revealed conceptions and strategies that students used to construct their arrangements and make comparisons. In general, students moved from simple, one-dimensional understandings of the standard deviation that did not consider<br>variation about the mean to more mean-centered conceptualizations that coordinated the effects of frequency (density) and deviation from the mean. Discussions of the results and implications for instruction and further research are presented.
  • Author(s):
    delMas, R. &amp; Liu, Y.
    Editors:
    Lee, C. &amp; Satterlee, A.
    Year:
    2003
    Abstract:
    This study investigated students' understanding of the concept of the standard deviation. In particular, students' understanding of the factors that affect, and how they affect, the size of the standard deviation were investigated. Thirteen students enrolled in an introductory statistics course participated in the study. Students engaged in two activities during a one-hour interview. In the first activity, they arranged a number of bars on a number line to produce the largest and smallest standard deviation. The second activity asked students to judge the relative sizes of the standard deviation of two distributions. Initial analysis identified rules/strategies that students used to construct their arrangements and make comparisons. A discussion of these distinctive rules and the conceptions they represent is presented.
  • Author(s):
    Lesser, L. M.
    Year:
    1999
    Abstract:
    The Birthday Problem is "How many people must be in a room before the probability that some share a birthday (ignoring the year and ignoring leap days) becomes at least 50%?" Multiple approaches to the problem are explored and compared, addressing probability concepts, problem solving, modelling assumptions, approximations (supported by Taylor series), recursion, (Excel) spreadsheets, simulation, and student preconceptions. The traditional product representation that yields the exact answer is not only tedious with a regular calculator, but did not provide insight on why the answer (23) is so much smaller than most students' predictions (typically, half of 365). A more intuitive (but slightly inexact) approach synthesized by the author focuses on the total number of "opportunities" for matched birthdays (e.g., the new "opportunities" for a match added by the kth person who enters are those that the kth person has with each of the k-1 people already there). The author followed the model of Shaughnessy (1977) in having students give predictions in advance of the exploration and these written data (as well as interview data) collected from students indicated representative multiplier or representative quotient effects, consistent with the literature on misconceptions and heuristics. Data collected from students after the traditional and "opportunities" explorations indicate that a majority of students preferred the opportunities approach, favoring the large gain in intuition over the slight loss in precision.
  • Author(s):
    Quinn, R. J.
    Editors:
    Goodall, G.
    Year:
    2003
    Abstract:
    Summary This article discusses three probabilistic scenarios based on the television game show 'Who Wants to be a Millionaire?'. These situations provide motivational opportunities for high-school students to explore the concepts of expected value, permutations and independent events.
  • Author(s):
    delMas, R. C., Garfield, J., &amp; Chance, B. L.
    Year:
    1999
    Abstract:
    The Sampling Distributions program and ancillary instructional materials were developed to guide student exploration and discovery. The program provides graphical, visual feedback which allows students to construct their own understanding of sampling distribution behavior. Diagnostic, graphics-based test items were developed to capture students' conceptual understanding before and after use of the program. Several versions of the activity have been used to date with mixed results. Our findings demonstrate that while software can provide the means for a rich classroom experience, computer simulations alone do not guarantee conceptual change.

Pages

The CAUSE Research Group is supported in part by a member initiative grant from the American Statistical Association’s Section on Statistics and Data Science Education