Chance News 14: Difference between revisions
mNo edit summary |
|||
(30 intermediate revisions by 5 users not shown) | |||
Line 5: | Line 5: | ||
==Forsooth== | ==Forsooth== | ||
Line 11: | Line 10: | ||
</blockquote> | </blockquote> | ||
<div align="right">Brian Rockey, | <div align="right">Brian Rockey, Nebraska Lottery Spokesman.</div><br> | ||
---- | |||
Who do we believe? | |||
"Two widely used nutritional supplements for arthritis pain do not effectively soothe patients' aching arthritic knees, a large federal study has found." | |||
--NYT, February 23, 2006 | |||
"A combination of the popular dietary supplements glucosamine and chondroitin sulfate appears to relieve knee pain associated with moderate-to-severe arthritis, according to a large federally funded study." | |||
--WSJ, February 23, 2006 | |||
---- | |||
"No effect was found for glucosamine, chondroitin or a combination of the two." | |||
--NYT | |||
"Patients who had more pain did seem to be helped by the combination." | |||
--Dr. Daniel Clegg, lead author of the study | |||
---- | |||
"It's a null trial. It doesn't work any better than placebo." | |||
--Dr. David Felson, a Boston University rheumatologist | |||
"I am going to continue doing it." | |||
--Nancy MacLeod, a user of the supplement | |||
---- | |||
"This is a spurious subset result if I've ever seen one. I wouldn't spend a nickel trying to confirm it." | |||
--Dr. Donald Berry, M.D. Anderson Cancer Center | |||
"If I had severe pain from osteoarthritis of the knee, based on this study, I would try glucosamine and chondroitin sulfate." | |||
--Dr. M. Elizabeth Halloran, biostatistics professor at the Fred Hutchinson Cancer Research Center and the University of Washington | |||
---- | |||
"Dr. Halloran said she was swayed not only by the data but also by her sister's experience giving the supplements to her arthritic dog." | |||
-- NYT, February 23, 2006 | |||
"But, arthritis researchers say, they know of no biological reason why eating those compounds would help people with arthritis." | |||
-- NYT, February 23, 2006 | |||
---- | |||
Submitted by Paul Alper. | |||
==Gerd Gigerenzer's Calculated Risks Revisited== | ==Gerd Gigerenzer's Calculated Risks Revisited== | ||
[http://www.dartmouth.edu/%7Echance/chance_news/recent_news/chance_news_11.03.html#item10 Chance News 11.03] had a lengthy and very positive review of Gerd Gigerenzer's book '''Calculated Risks: How To Know When Numbers Deceive You'''. | [http://www.dartmouth.edu/%7Echance/chance_news/recent_news/chance_news_11.03.html#item10 Chance News 11.03] had a lengthy and very positive review of Gerd Gigerenzer's book '''Calculated Risks: How To Know When Numbers Deceive You'''. | ||
Readers are urged to download that excellent review because of the information contained. However, the book is so good and so persuasive that it is worth another look in order to alert readers to some other aspects of the book and how it relates to subsequent events. | Readers are urged to download that excellent review because of the information contained. However, the book is so good and so persuasive that it is worth another look in order to alert readers to some other aspects of the book and how it relates to subsequent events. | ||
The aforementioned review did not mention the abundant number of actual, real-world incidents cited in which doctors, lawyers and social workers, not to mention patients, clients and jurors, were unable to unscramble the difference between P(X| Y) and P(Y| X). Also not mentioned was Gigerenzer's dim view of screening for breast cancer and prostate cancer. Screening may be defined, according to H. Gilbert Welch, as "the systematic examination of asymptomatic people to detect and treat disease." See [http://chance.dartmouth.edu/chancewiki/index.php/Chance_News_12#Screening Chance News 12] for a review of Welch's 2004 book '''Should I Be Tested For Cancer? Maybe Not And Here's Why'''. Welch echoes and amplifies Gigerenzer contention that (mass) screening is counterproductive, especially when there is little evidence that a cure exists. Just to complicate matters, however, see [http://chance.dartmouth.edu/chancewiki/index.php/Chance_News_8#Mammograms_Validated_as_Key_in_Cancer_Fight "Mammograms validated as key in cancer fight"] in Chance News 8 which indicates that mammography screening does reduce the death rate of breast cancer, . Unfortunately, the article in the ''New England Journal of Medicine'' referred to does not explain why mammogram screening is deemed responsible for 28 to 65% of the 24% drop in the breast cancer death rate. | The aforementioned review did not mention the abundant number of actual, real-world incidents cited in which doctors, lawyers and social workers, not to mention patients, clients and jurors, were unable to unscramble the difference between P(X| Y) and P(Y| X). Also not mentioned was Gigerenzer's dim view of screening for breast cancer and prostate cancer. Screening may be defined, according to H. Gilbert Welch, as "the systematic examination of asymptomatic people to detect and treat disease." See [http://chance.dartmouth.edu/chancewiki/index.php/Chance_News_12#Screening Chance News 12] for a review of Welch's 2004 book '''Should I Be Tested For Cancer? Maybe Not And Here's Why'''. Welch echoes and amplifies Gigerenzer contention that (mass) screening is counterproductive, especially when there is little evidence that a cure exists. Just to complicate matters, however, see [http://chance.dartmouth.edu/chancewiki/index.php/Chance_News_8#Mammograms_Validated_as_Key_in_Cancer_Fight "Mammograms validated as key in cancer fight"] in Chance News 8 which indicates that mammography screening does reduce the death rate of breast cancer, . Unfortunately, the article in the ''New England Journal of Medicine'' referred to does not explain why mammogram screening is deemed responsible for 28 to 65% of the 24% drop in the breast cancer death rate. | ||
Line 87: | Line 120: | ||
Submitted by Paul Alper | Submitted by Paul Alper | ||
==More medical studies | ==More medical studies that conflict with previous studies== | ||
[http://www.nytimes.com/2006/02/08/health/08fat.html?_r=1&oref=slogin Low-fat diet does not cut health risks, Study finds]<br> | [http://www.nytimes.com/2006/02/08/health/08fat.html?_r=1&oref=slogin Low-fat diet does not cut health risks, Study finds]<br> | ||
Line 152: | Line 185: | ||
* [http://blog.proteinpower.com/drmike/archives/2006/02/man_bites_dog.html Man Bites Dog] and [http://blog.proteinpower.com/drmike/archives/2006/02/man_bites_dog_i.html Man Bites Dog II], Michael R. Eades, M.D. offers a critical review of the design of the study. | * [http://blog.proteinpower.com/drmike/archives/2006/02/man_bites_dog.html Man Bites Dog] and [http://blog.proteinpower.com/drmike/archives/2006/02/man_bites_dog_i.html Man Bites Dog II], Michael R. Eades, M.D. offers a critical review of the design of the study. | ||
* [http://www.weightoftheevidence.com/ Regina Wilshire's blog] on why we don't need more time and/or more studies to 'prove' that low-fat dieting really works. | * [http://www.weightoftheevidence.com/ Regina Wilshire's blog] on why we don't need more time and/or more studies to 'prove' that low-fat dieting really works. | ||
* [http://www.stat.columbia.edu/~cook/movabletype/archives/2006/02/do_lowfat_diets_1.html Do low-fat diets have "significant" benefits?] in Andrew Gelman's blog discusses the rejection levels and how a slight change in the data (or the rejection level) would have converted an "insignificant" result into a "significant" one in this particular case. | |||
==A day in the life of a human rights statistician== | ==A day in the life of a human rights statistician== | ||
Line 158: | Line 192: | ||
A group of determined programmers and statisticians, | A group of determined programmers and statisticians, | ||
the [http://www.hrdag.org/about/ Human Rights Data Analysis Group], released a [http://www.hrdag.org/resources/timor_chapter_graphs/timor_chapter_page_01.shtml report] documenting | the [http://www.hrdag.org/about/ Human Rights Data Analysis Group], released a [http://www.hrdag.org/resources/timor_chapter_graphs/timor_chapter_page_01.shtml report] documenting civilian deaths in the former Portuguese colony of Timor-Leste, which occurred from a year prior to the Indonesian army's invasion in 1975, to the country's 1999 independence referendum that formally ended the occupation. | ||
[http://www.hrdag.org/about/timor-leste.shtml Statistical analysis] establishes that at least 102,800 (+/- 11,000) Timorese died as a result of the conflict. Approximately 18,600 (+/- 1000) Timorese were killed or disappeared, while the remainder died due to hunger and illness in excess of what would be expected due to peacetime mortality. | [http://www.hrdag.org/about/timor-leste.shtml Statistical analysis] establishes that at least 102,800 (+/- 11,000) Timorese died as a result of the conflict. Approximately 18,600 (+/- 1000) Timorese were killed or disappeared, while the remainder died due to hunger and illness in excess of what would be expected due to peacetime mortality. | ||
Line 207: | Line 241: | ||
==Another record jackpot for the Powerball lottery== | ==Another record jackpot for the Powerball lottery== | ||
[http://www.startribune.com/535/story/247617.html | [http://www.startribune.com/535/story/247617.html Elusiveness of Powerball is revealed in the math]<br> | ||
Minneapolis Star Tribune, Feb. 15, 2006<br> | Minneapolis Star Tribune, Feb. 15, 2006<br> | ||
Mike Meyers | Mike Meyers | ||
[http://www.latimes.com/news/ | [http://www.latimes.com/news/opinion/commentary/la-oe-daum25feb25,0,1184552.column?coll=la-news-comment-opinions Who's the idiot now?]<br> | ||
''Los Angeles Times'', Feb. 25, 2006<br> | ''Los Angeles Times'', Feb. 25, 2006<br> | ||
Meghan Doum. | Meghan Doum. | ||
When a | When a Powerball jackpot nears a new record the media asks experts to comment on what the odds are and to explain how unlikely you are to win a lottery. See [http://chance.dartmouth.edu/chancewiki/index.php/Chance_News_8#A_record_Powerball_lottery_jackpot Chance News 8] for interesting comments of two Minnesota mathematicians related to the October 22, 2005 record $340 million Powerball jackpot. For his article related to the current record $365 jackpot, Meyers consulted [http://www.math.temple.edu/~paulos/ John Paulos] author of the bestselling book, "Innumeracy" and a monthly column [http://abcnews.go.com/Technology/WhosCounting/story?id=1560771 Who's Counting] for ABCNews.com. | ||
We read: | We read: | ||
Line 223: | Line 257: | ||
Paulos once tore up a Powerball ticket on the eve of a drawing in front of an audience. "They all gasped as if I just slashed the Mona Lisa," he said. </blockquote> | Paulos once tore up a Powerball ticket on the eve of a drawing in front of an audience. "They all gasped as if I just slashed the Mona Lisa," he said. </blockquote> | ||
While the Voltaire quote is on many websites and usually attributed to Voltaire our librarians were unable to find | While the Voltaire quote is on many websites and usually attributed to Voltaire our librarians were unable to find its source. Perhaps a reader can provide this. However in our search we did find a similar quotation: | ||
<center>A lottery is a taxation, Upon all the fools in Creation;<br> | <center>A lottery is a taxation, Upon all the fools in Creation;<br> | ||
Line 230: | Line 264: | ||
For, folly's a fund, Will never lose ground,<br> | For, folly's a fund, Will never lose ground,<br> | ||
While fools are so rife in the Nation.</center> | While fools are so rife in the Nation.</center> | ||
<div align="right">Henry | <div align="right">Henry Fielding 1707-54<br> | ||
</div> | </div> | ||
From Wikipedia we read: | From Wikipedia we read: | ||
<blockquote>'''Henry Fielding''' was an English | <blockquote>'''Henry Fielding''' was an English novelist and dramatist known for his rich earthy humor and satirical prowess and as the author of the novel ''Tom Jones''.</blockquote> | ||
The quotation comes from Fielding's play [http://www.ebookmall.com/ebook/169052-ebook.htm ''The Lottery, a farce''] (1724) | The quotation comes from Fielding's play [http://www.ebookmall.com/ebook/169052-ebook.htm ''The Lottery, a farce''] (1724) | ||
In her ''Los | In her ''Los Angeles Times'' article [http://www.latimes.com/news/opinion/commentary/la-oe-daum25feb25,0,1184552.column?coll=la-news-comment-opinions Who's the idiot now?] about the winners of the current record lottery columnist Meghan Daum writes: | ||
<blockquote>On Wednesday morning in Lincoln, Neb., after four days of speculation about who had won the biggest jackpot in Powerball history, eight employees of a ConAgra ham processing plant came forward and identified themselves as the winners of the $365-million purse. As lottery stories go, this is about as heartwarming as it gets. Two of the winners are immigrants from Vietnam and one is a political refugee from the Republic of Congo -- and all worked the second and third shifts, some clocking as many as 70 hours a week. There is probably no jobsite as gruesome as a meatpacking house. If anyone deserves an express ticket to a new life, it's these folks. </blockquote> | <blockquote>On Wednesday morning in Lincoln, Neb., after four days of speculation about who had won the biggest jackpot in Powerball history, eight employees of a ConAgra ham processing plant came forward and identified themselves as the winners of the $365-million purse. As lottery stories go, this is about as heartwarming as it gets. Two of the winners are immigrants from Vietnam and one is a political refugee from the Republic of Congo -- and all worked the second and third shifts, some clocking as many as 70 hours a week. There is probably no jobsite as gruesome as a meatpacking house. If anyone deserves an express ticket to a new life, it's these folks. </blockquote> | ||
Equally moving is | Equally moving is the [http://www.cnn.com/2006/US/02/22/powerball.winner.ap/ interview] of the winners on CNN. Click on "Watch presentation of big checks" in the article). (You have to watch a short advertisement first). | ||
===Other sources=== | |||
[http://www.slate.com/id/114577/ Is powerball a mug's game?] ''Slate Magazine'', Jordon Ellenberg. | |||
[http://www.dartmouth.edu/~chance/teaching_aids/profiles.html Using lotteries in teaching chance], Bill Peterson and Laurie Snell. | |||
Both of these discuss the problem of determining when the lottery is a favorable game. Both need updating because of the coninuing changes in the powerball lottery. | |||
==Europe's statisticians are too gloomy== | ==Europe's statisticians are too gloomy== | ||
Line 265: | Line 308: | ||
</blockquote> | </blockquote> | ||
This article is based on a paper by Kevin Daly, an economist at Goldman Sachs. | This article is based on a paper by Kevin Daly, an economist at Goldman Sachs, an Investment Bank. | ||
He calculates that, based on the GDP-growth figures first published in each quarter, the euro area would have grown by an annual average of only 1.6% in the six years to 2004. Yet the latest figures put the growth rate at 2.0%. | He calculates that, based on the GDP-growth figures first published in each quarter, the euro area would have grown by an annual average of only 1.6% in the six years to 2004. Yet the latest figures put the growth rate at 2.0%. | ||
In contrast, the first published figures gave America an average growth rate of 3.1%; but that has now been shaved down to 2.8%. The revisions have cut the reported gap between growth rates in America and the euro area in half. As a result, the euro area's GDP per head has in fact grown at the same pace as America's. | In contrast, the first published figures gave America an average growth rate of 3.1%; but that has now been shaved down to 2.8%. The revisions have cut the reported gap between growth rates in America and the euro area in half. As a result, the euro area's GDP per head has in fact grown at the same pace as America's. |
Latest revision as of 13:41, 6 May 2008
Quotation
The [Supreme] Court concluded that mental health professionals' predictions were "not always wrong...only most of the time."
Forsooth
In theory, if you were to buy 50 tickets and your neighbor bought one, neither of you would have a better or worse chance of winning, We like to say it only takes one ticket to win."
Who do we believe?
"Two widely used nutritional supplements for arthritis pain do not effectively soothe patients' aching arthritic knees, a large federal study has found." --NYT, February 23, 2006
"A combination of the popular dietary supplements glucosamine and chondroitin sulfate appears to relieve knee pain associated with moderate-to-severe arthritis, according to a large federally funded study." --WSJ, February 23, 2006
"No effect was found for glucosamine, chondroitin or a combination of the two." --NYT
"Patients who had more pain did seem to be helped by the combination." --Dr. Daniel Clegg, lead author of the study
"It's a null trial. It doesn't work any better than placebo." --Dr. David Felson, a Boston University rheumatologist
"I am going to continue doing it." --Nancy MacLeod, a user of the supplement
"This is a spurious subset result if I've ever seen one. I wouldn't spend a nickel trying to confirm it." --Dr. Donald Berry, M.D. Anderson Cancer Center
"If I had severe pain from osteoarthritis of the knee, based on this study, I would try glucosamine and chondroitin sulfate." --Dr. M. Elizabeth Halloran, biostatistics professor at the Fred Hutchinson Cancer Research Center and the University of Washington
"Dr. Halloran said she was swayed not only by the data but also by her sister's experience giving the supplements to her arthritic dog." -- NYT, February 23, 2006
"But, arthritis researchers say, they know of no biological reason why eating those compounds would help people with arthritis." -- NYT, February 23, 2006
Submitted by Paul Alper.
Gerd Gigerenzer's Calculated Risks Revisited
Chance News 11.03 had a lengthy and very positive review of Gerd Gigerenzer's book Calculated Risks: How To Know When Numbers Deceive You. Readers are urged to download that excellent review because of the information contained. However, the book is so good and so persuasive that it is worth another look in order to alert readers to some other aspects of the book and how it relates to subsequent events.
The aforementioned review did not mention the abundant number of actual, real-world incidents cited in which doctors, lawyers and social workers, not to mention patients, clients and jurors, were unable to unscramble the difference between P(X| Y) and P(Y| X). Also not mentioned was Gigerenzer's dim view of screening for breast cancer and prostate cancer. Screening may be defined, according to H. Gilbert Welch, as "the systematic examination of asymptomatic people to detect and treat disease." See Chance News 12 for a review of Welch's 2004 book Should I Be Tested For Cancer? Maybe Not And Here's Why. Welch echoes and amplifies Gigerenzer contention that (mass) screening is counterproductive, especially when there is little evidence that a cure exists. Just to complicate matters, however, see "Mammograms validated as key in cancer fight" in Chance News 8 which indicates that mammography screening does reduce the death rate of breast cancer, . Unfortunately, the article in the New England Journal of Medicine referred to does not explain why mammogram screening is deemed responsible for 28 to 65% of the 24% drop in the breast cancer death rate. Gigerenzer would prefer, and this is one of his main points, that any statistical data be given in counts rather than in percentages, especially percentages without a base rate, such as relative risk which he views as the most misleading.
Treatment | Deaths per 1000 women |
---|---|
No mammography screening | 4 |
Mammography screening | 3 |
Consequently, there is "a 25 percent relative risk reduction." He would prefer focusing on the difference in the number of deaths which yields the more revealing and perhaps more honest statement: "The absolute risk reduction is 4 minus 3, that is, 1 out of 1000 women (which corresponds to .1 percent)." However, "Counting on their clients' innumeracy, organizations that want to impress upon clients the benefits of treatment generally report them in terms of relative risk reduction...applicants [for grants] often feel compelled to report relative risk reductions because they sound more impressive." Although he did not use this example, one's relative "risk" of winning the lottery is infinitely greater if one buys a ticket, yet one's absolute "risk" of winning has hardly improved at all.
Most of his numerical examples are typified by his discussion of the cartoon given below
which indicates the superiority of dealing with counts. Note that "H" represents having the disease and "D" represents a diagnosis having the symptom as seen by testing positive. Characteristically, there is a large number in the population who do not have the disease and because of the possibility of a wrong classification, the number of false positives (99) outweighs the number of true positives (8) resulting in P(disease| symptom) being much lower (8/(8+99)) than P(symptom| disease) (.8). This type of result, low probability of disease given symptom, is true even when ".8" is replaced by a number much closer to 1 provided there are many more who do not have the disease.
Here is an example he did not consider but it also illustrates the superiority of dealing with counts. Instead of two populations--diseased and healthy--which are greatly different in size, consider Boys and Girls and the desire to predict gender based on some simple test. Assume that 50% of births are Boys so that P(Boy) = P(Girl) = 1/2. A simple, inexpensive, non-invasive gender-testing procedure indicates that it is "perfect" for boys, P(Test Boy| Boy) = 1, implying P(Test Girl| Boy) = 0. Unfortunately, this simple, inexpensive, non-invasive gender-testing procedure for girls is a "coin toss," P(Test Girl| Girl) = P(Test Boy| Girl) = 1/2. Application of Bayes theorem yields what seems to be a strange inversion, P(Boy| Test Boy) = 2/3 and P(Girl| Test Girl) = 1. That is, somehow, "perfection" switched from Boy to Girl. The test is perfect in "confirming" that a Boy is a Boy and has a 50% error rate in confirming that a Girl is a Girl. The test is perfect in "predicting" that a person who tests as a girl is in fact a girl but has 33% error rate in predicting that a person who tests as a Boy is in fact a Boy. Thus, the term perfect is ambiguous. Perfection in confirmation, i.e., the test conditional on the gender, does not mean perfection in prediction, i.e., the gender conditional on the test.
Some of the puzzlement disappears if we deal with counts; the table below is equivalent to Gigerenzer's "tree" diagram. Assume 50 Boys and 50 Girls to start with. Every one of the 50 Boys will test as a Boy--none of the Boys test as a Girl; of the 50 Girls, 25 will test as a Boy and 25 will test as a girl. Therefore, P(Girl| Test Girl) = 1. One is tempted to to explain the switch by using the lingo of medical testing: false positives, false negatives, sensitivity, specificity, positive predictive value, negative predictive value. However, one hesitates to designate either gender as diseased even though the mathematics is the same.
Test Boy | Test Girl | Total | |
---|---|---|---|
Boy | 50 | 0 | 50 |
Girl | 25 | 25 | 50 |
Gigerenzer rightly concludes that the language of statistics is not natural for most individuals. Perhaps the puzzlement in this specific example is at least partly due to the natural language known as English. Boys, Girls, Test Boys and Test Girls are too confusing. . Replace "Boy" by "Norwegian" and "Girl" by "German" and assume that there are as many Norwegians as Germans. Let every Norwegian be "Blond," so that P(Blond| Norwegian) = 1 and only half the Germans are Blond. Thus, P(German| Not Blond) =1; the switch, P(German| Not Blond) = P(Blond| Norwegian) = 1, is rather obvious. Is the this situation easier to understand because of the linguistics--hair color and ethnicity are easily distinct as Test Boy and Boy are not?
DISCUSSION QUESTIONS
1. Gigerenzer has a chapter entitled, "(Un)Informed Consent." Based on your experience, what do you imagine the chapter contains?
2. A drawing of two tables (that is, physical tables on which things are placed) appears on page 10. He claims the tables (due to Roger Shepard) are identical in size and shape. After staring at them in disbelief of the claim, how would you verify the contention?
3. Physicians sometime make the following type of statement:"Never mind the statistics, I treat every patient as an individual." Defend this assertion. Criticize this assertion.
4. The physicist, Lord Rutherford, is reputed to have said, " If your experiment, needs statistics you ought to have done a better experiment." Defend and criticize Lord Rutherford.
5. Assume an asymptomatic woman has a mammogram which looks suspicious and then a biopsy which is negative. Would she be grateful for the clean bill of health or would she become an advocate who opposes (mass) screening? Suppose instead we assume a man has a suspiciously high PSA and the painful multiple biopsies (6-12 "sticks") are all negative. Would he be grateful for the clean bill of health or would he become an advocate who opposes (mass) screening?
6. Calculated Risks also deals with the risk to the physician making a recommendation and a diagnosis. Discuss why in our present-day litigious society the risks to the physician (who may or may not recommend a test or may or may not make a diagnosis) are not symmetrical. Along these lines, who are the vested interests involved in maintaining screening and testing?
7. Revisit the Boy/Girl scenario but now the test always says Boy regardless of gender, P(Test Boy| Boy) = P(Test Boy| Girl) = 1. Complete the table for this version. Obviously, this test has the advantage of being extremely simple, cost-free and non-invasive. Use either the Probability Format or the Frequency Format to comment on the statistical worthiness of this test.
Submitted by Paul Alper
More medical studies that conflict with previous studies
Low-fat diet does not cut health risks, Study finds
New York Times, Feb. 8, 2006
Gina Kolata
Cutting fat alone isn't enough, women advised
USA TODAY, Feb, 7, 2006
Rita Rubin
Popular herb shows no benefit for prostate
Wall street journal, Feb. 9, 2006
Syllvia Pagan Westphal
In the New York Times article we read:
The largest study ever to ask whether a low-fat diet reduces the risk of getting cancer or heart disease has found that the diet has no effect.
The $415 million federal study involved nearly 49,000 women ages 50 to 79 who were followed for eight years. In the end, those assigned to a low-fat diet had the same rates of breast cancer, colon cancer, heart attacks and strokes as those who ate whatever they pleased, researchers are reporting today.
In the Wall Street Journal article we read:
Saw palmetto, an herbal supplement taken by 2.5 million Americans for problems with enlargement of the prostate gland, is no more effective than a placebo in alleviating the condition, according to a new study
The perception that saw palmetto works had been supported by a number of clinical trials over the years. A comprehensive 2002 analysis of 21 trials involving over 3,000 men found that studies credited saw palmetto with providing "mild to moderate improvement in symptoms with fewer adverse events than finasteride (approved by the Food and Drug Administration to treat benign enlargement).
The saw palmetto study was reported in the New England Journal of Medicine February 9, 2006 and the low-fat diet study was reported in Jama, February 8, 2006.
The saw palmetto study had 225 participants randomized to give 112 saw palmetto and 113 placebo and the study lasted from July 2001 to May 2003.
So once more the general public will wonder what the truth is. Commenting on the diet study Berkeley statistician David Freedmn is quoted as saying that the studies were well designed and should be taken seriously.
Two of the 32 authors of the study, Judith Hsia, professor of medicine at George Washington University in Washington, D.C. and Ross Prentice, professor of biostatistics at the University of Washington in Seattle, were interviewed by Ira Flato on NPR's Talk of the Nation Friday Science program February 10, 2006. You can listen to them trying to do damage control here . The say that women should certainly not quit trying to control their diet. They point out that the study did not distinquish between "good" and "bad" fats. Prentice remarks that the incidence rate of breast cancer was 9% less in control group than in the placebo group. When Ira remarks that the study said that the difference was not significant, Prentice replied that you have to understand what statistical signficance meant and adds that if it were 10% it would have been signficant.
The authors of the paper also comment that the study was not able to continue for the length of time originally planned and since the trend for the incidence of Breast cancer was in the right direction so it is possible that the difference might be significant over the longer time. Here is a graphic from the Jama article that shows the difference in the incident rates over time http://www.dartmouth.edu/~chance/chance_news/for_chance_news/wiki/diet.png.
On Feb. 14 Kolata wrote in the Times a sequal Maybe you're not what you eat to her Feb 8 article where she attempts to explain the conflicting views of the results of the Women's study. You will find here her answers to readers questions relating to her Feb. 14 article.
Then in the Times on Feb. 16 she wrote an article Big study finds no clear benefit of calcium pills about another study based on the Women's study. She writes:
The $18 million study was part of the Women's Health Initiative, a large federal project whose results have confounded some popular beliefs and raised questions about public health messages that had been addressed to the entire population.
In the new study, the participants were randomly assigned to take 1,000 milligrams of calcium and 400 international units of vitamin D a day, or to take placebos, and were followed for seven years. Researchers looked for effects on bone density, fractures and colorectal cancer. The lack of an effect on colorectal cancer over the seven years was so clear that it has aroused little debate. But the effect on bones is another story.
Osteoporosis specialists said the study, published today in the New England Journal of Medicine, was likely to put a dent in what has become a widespread medical practice of recommending that all women take calcium and vitamin D supplements starting at menopause if not sooner, as a sort of insurance policy against osteoporosis. But beyond that there is no agreement on what, if anything, healthy women should do.
This led to still another New York Times article by Denise Grady Feb. 19, 2006 Women's health sudies leaves questions in place of certainty This article begins with:
So what do women do now? The results of two major studies over the past two weeks have questioned the value of two widely recommended measures: calcium pills and vitamin D to prevent broken bones, and low-fat diets to ward off heart disease and breast and colon cancer.
The article discusses the conflicts between statisticians who are willing to accept the outcomes of the study and researchers who want to look at subgroups to try to argue that despite the lack of significance one can see hopeful signs. Statistician Susan Ellenberg remarks:
The probability that you will see a spuriously positive effect gets very big very quickly.
Ellenberg quotes another statistician, Richard Peto of Oxford University, who said of subgroups:
You should always do them but you should never believe them.
You will also find in this article a nice graphic summerizing the results of the Women's study relating to low-fact diets and vitamin D.
Further reading
The low-fat diet study has attracted a lot of attention from bloggers.
- Man Bites Dog and Man Bites Dog II, Michael R. Eades, M.D. offers a critical review of the design of the study.
- Regina Wilshire's blog on why we don't need more time and/or more studies to 'prove' that low-fat dieting really works.
- Do low-fat diets have "significant" benefits? in Andrew Gelman's blog discusses the rejection levels and how a slight change in the data (or the rejection level) would have converted an "insignificant" result into a "significant" one in this particular case.
A day in the life of a human rights statistician
Coders Bare Invasion Death Count, By Ann Harrison, Wired News, 9-Feb-06.
How statistics caught Indonesia's war-criminals , Cory Doctorow, BoingBoing.net
A group of determined programmers and statisticians, the Human Rights Data Analysis Group, released a report documenting civilian deaths in the former Portuguese colony of Timor-Leste, which occurred from a year prior to the Indonesian army's invasion in 1975, to the country's 1999 independence referendum that formally ended the occupation. Statistical analysis establishes that at least 102,800 (+/- 11,000) Timorese died as a result of the conflict. Approximately 18,600 (+/- 1000) Timorese were killed or disappeared, while the remainder died due to hunger and illness in excess of what would be expected due to peacetime mortality.
Group director Patrick Ball says
By having an accurate statistical picture of the suffering, we can draw conclusions about what the causes of the violence might have been and identify likely perpetrators with a claim based on thousands of witnesses.
The group established three datasets that integrated quantitative methods into broader truth seeking activities. These datasets included:
- The commission's statement-taking process, which collected almost 8,000 narrative testimonies from people in every sub-district;
- A census of all public graveyards in the country (encompassing approximately 319,000 gravestones);
- A retrospective mortality survey drawing on a probability sample of approximately 1,400 households throughout the thirteen districts of Timor-Leste.
In establishing these data, HRDAG and the Commission for Reception, Truth and Reconciliation in East Timor (CAVR) pioneered a number of new techniques and methods. No other truth commission has ever undertaken a retrospective mortality survey. While gravestone information for mortality estimation has been used by historical demographers for mortality estimations, this is the first time that a human rights project has employed such methods. These projects were so large that HRDAG developed automated techniques to link multiple reports of the same death - a key component of multiple systems estimation, a technique that uses two separately collected but incomplete lists of a population to estimate the total population size.
HRDAG uses the multiple systems estimation technique in human rights cases to project the total number of violations, including those that were never documented. This information is vital to producing a complete accurate historical record of the violations and to provide evidence at the trial of the architects of large-scale human rights abuses. In order to make statistical inferences from multiple systems estimation, it is necessary to:
- Identify overlapping reports
- Control for bias and variation in coverage rates
- Estimate the total magnitude
Ball has spent the last 15 years building systems and conducting qualitative analysis for large-scale human rights data projects around the world. HRGAD researchers used comparative analysis of the datasets to uncover patterns of deaths and build objective evidence of abuses. The team also developed an array of descriptive statistical analysis profiling the scale, pattern and structure of torture, ill-treatment, arbitrary detention and sexual violations. In order to estimate what was missing from the data, the HRDAG developed software to link multiple reports of the same death in a technique called record linkage. They then used multiple systems estimation to calculate the number of deaths that no one remembered.
Romesh Silva, a HRDAG field statistician who led the design and implementation of the project's data, says
The Indonesian military has persistently argued that excess mortality in Timor due to its occupation of Timor was zero. This claim can now be tested empirically and transparently with the tools of science instead of merely being debated with the tools of political rhetoric.
The final report of the CAVR was handed over to the President of Timor-Leste on 31 October 2005. The President of Timor-Leste then tabled the report at a special sitting of Timor-Leste's National Parliament on 28 November, 2005 - which coincided with the 30th anniversary celebrations of Timor's Proclamation of Independence.
Further reading
- The Human Rights Data Analysis Group (HRDAG) develops information technology solutions and statistical techniques to help human rights advocates build evidence-based arguments.] See the FAQ for more info.
- The Profile of Human Rights Violations in Timor-Leste, 1974-1999. A Report by the Benetech Human Rights Data Analysis Group to the Commission on Reception, Truth and Reconciliation of Timor-Leste. 9 February 2006.
- Romesh Silva and Patrick Ball designed and conducted the statistical analysis and wrote this report. Their on-line profiles provide more information about their statistical work and the awards that they have received. The are also profiles of other statistical consultants at HRDAG.
Two short papers by Romesh and Ball are worth reading:
- The Demography of Large-Scale Human Rights Atrocities: Integrating demographic and statistical analysis into post-conflict historical clarification in Timor-Leste. Romesh Silva and Patrick Ball. A 5-page paper presented at the 2006 meetings of the Population Association of America.
- My brilliant career - Quantitative Data Analysis and Large-Scale Human Rights Violations: An Example of Applied Statistics at the Grassroots. Romesh Silva. Gazette of the Australian Mathematical Society. Canberra (Australia). Volume 32, Number 2, May 2005. A 5-page paper on where a mathematics education can lead you.
- "in the late 1990’s as I was completing my honors degree in statistics at UNSW, I became increasingly involved with Amnesty International ... I began to wonder how I might be able to directly apply my mathematical education and quantitative skills to international human rights. ... As the field of human rights statistics is relatively new, our main challenge remains in attracting more mathematicians and statisticians to work in this area and also communicating to the human rights field how quantitative data analysis can contribute to accountability and truthtelling processes."
Submitted by John Gavin.
Another record jackpot for the Powerball lottery
Elusiveness of Powerball is revealed in the math
Minneapolis Star Tribune, Feb. 15, 2006
Mike Meyers
Who's the idiot now?
Los Angeles Times, Feb. 25, 2006
Meghan Doum.
When a Powerball jackpot nears a new record the media asks experts to comment on what the odds are and to explain how unlikely you are to win a lottery. See Chance News 8 for interesting comments of two Minnesota mathematicians related to the October 22, 2005 record $340 million Powerball jackpot. For his article related to the current record $365 jackpot, Meyers consulted John Paulos author of the bestselling book, "Innumeracy" and a monthly column Who's Counting for ABCNews.com.
We read:
Paulos says lotteries have always owed their appeal to people's loose grip of math and recalled a line from Voltaire: "Lotteries are a tax on stupidity."
Paulos once tore up a Powerball ticket on the eve of a drawing in front of an audience. "They all gasped as if I just slashed the Mona Lisa," he said.
While the Voltaire quote is on many websites and usually attributed to Voltaire our librarians were unable to find its source. Perhaps a reader can provide this. However in our search we did find a similar quotation:
And Heav'n be prais'd, It is easily rais'd,
Credulity's always in fashion;
For, folly's a fund, Will never lose ground,
From Wikipedia we read:
Henry Fielding was an English novelist and dramatist known for his rich earthy humor and satirical prowess and as the author of the novel Tom Jones.
The quotation comes from Fielding's play The Lottery, a farce (1724)
In her Los Angeles Times article Who's the idiot now? about the winners of the current record lottery columnist Meghan Daum writes:
On Wednesday morning in Lincoln, Neb., after four days of speculation about who had won the biggest jackpot in Powerball history, eight employees of a ConAgra ham processing plant came forward and identified themselves as the winners of the $365-million purse. As lottery stories go, this is about as heartwarming as it gets. Two of the winners are immigrants from Vietnam and one is a political refugee from the Republic of Congo -- and all worked the second and third shifts, some clocking as many as 70 hours a week. There is probably no jobsite as gruesome as a meatpacking house. If anyone deserves an express ticket to a new life, it's these folks.
Equally moving is the interview of the winners on CNN. Click on "Watch presentation of big checks" in the article). (You have to watch a short advertisement first).
Other sources
Is powerball a mug's game? Slate Magazine, Jordon Ellenberg.
Using lotteries in teaching chance, Bill Peterson and Laurie Snell.
Both of these discuss the problem of determining when the lottery is a favorable game. Both need updating because of the coninuing changes in the powerball lottery.
Europe's statisticians are too gloomy
A numbers racket, The Economist, 18-Feb-06.
How to measure economies, The Economist, 9-Feb-2006 (subscription required).
The first article highlights how statistical biases can influence perceptions of economic growth. The second gives more information about the merits of gross domestic product (GDP) relative to other economic indicators.
GDP per head is the most commonly used measure of a country's success. It was primarily developed as a planning tool to measure productivity during World War II as it measures value of goods and services produced by the residents of a country. A nation's well-being depends on factors not covered by GDP, such as leisure time, income inequality and the quality of the envirnoment but GDP was never intended to measure welfare. For most purposes, it the best available indicator on a timely basis so governments worry about how to boost their GDP growth.
There is a wide gap between America's and Europe's GPD per head. Since the start of European Monetary Union in 1999, revisions to gross domestic product (GDP) growth have almost always been upwards. In contrast, revisions in America have tended to be downwards. The initial figures which grab newspaper headlines therefore exaggerate Europe's economic underperformance.
The Economist resists the obvious conclusion:
Discounting the obvious explanation that American statisticians are born optimists, it is unclear what lies behind the consistent direction of these revisions.
This article is based on a paper by Kevin Daly, an economist at Goldman Sachs, an Investment Bank. He calculates that, based on the GDP-growth figures first published in each quarter, the euro area would have grown by an annual average of only 1.6% in the six years to 2004. Yet the latest figures put the growth rate at 2.0%. In contrast, the first published figures gave America an average growth rate of 3.1%; but that has now been shaved down to 2.8%. The revisions have cut the reported gap between growth rates in America and the euro area in half. As a result, the euro area's GDP per head has in fact grown at the same pace as America's.
The Economist goes on to comment:
Europe could rejoice in further upward revisions to growth if its governments were to adopt American statistical practices. Price deflators there take more account of improvements in the quality of goods, such as computers, and thus a given rise in nominal spending implies faster growth in real terms. By using higher inflation rates, the euro area understates its growth relative to America's. In addition, American statisticians consider firms' spending on software that is written in-house to be investment, while in the euro area it is often counted as an expense and so is excluded from final output. The surge in software spending has therefore inflated America's relative growth.
On past experience, Europe's statisticians should add half a percentage point to their first guesses of GDP growth. By also switching to American practices, they could boost growth even further. Instead, their cautious ways are making Europe's economies look more dismal than they are, and gloomy headlines are discouraging consumers from spending. Perhaps Europe should outsource the compilation of its statistics to America, and then watch the boom.
The second Economist article says that the OECD is encouraging governments to move from relying on just one indicator. Alternatives, like gross or net national income suggest that the gap between American and European growth rates may much smaller.
Further reading
Has Euroland Performed That Badly?, Kevin Daly, Golman Sachs. Daly says
Euroland productivity when measured appropriately is not only close to US levels but, over the past ten years as a whole, its growth has continued to surpass that of the US. The US’s superior GDP performance over this period has not been attributable to faster productivity growth but to a more rapidly expanding labour force that is prepared to work longer hours.
Submitted by John Gavin.
Single and not so carefree
Premature mortality among lone fathers and childless men. Ringback Weitoft G, Burstrom B, Rosen M. Soc Sci Med. 2004 Oct;59(7):1449-59.
This study is a couple of years old, but it is interesting in itself and in how it was reported by a conservative advocacy group. I have not read the full article, so I can only comment on the abstract, which is available on PubMed.
These researchers studied 682,919 men and divided them into five groups (lone fathers with custody of their child/children, lone fathers without custody, childless men with a wife, childless men without a wife, and men with a wife and men with a wife and child/children. The last group was the comparison group for all comparisons. They analyzed deaths in these groups from 1991 to 2000.
"The results suggest that lone non-custodial fathers and lone childless men face the greatest increase in risks, especially from injury and addiction, and also from all-cause mortality and ischaemic heart disease. Being a lone custodial father also entails increased risk, although generally to a much lesser extent, and not for all outcomes. The elevated risks found in all the subgroups considered diminished substantially when proxy variables to control for health-selection effects and socioeconomic circumstances were added to the initial model. Risks fell most in response to introduction of the socioeconomic variables, but health selection also played a major role, mostly in the cases of lone non-custodial fathers and lone childless men. However, even following these adjustments, significant risk increases, although greatly attenuated, remained for all the subgroups."
No mention was made about adjustment for age, but this would have to be done because there is almost certainly a large disparity in the age of men with and without children.
I became aware of this paper when my brother-in-law sent me an email describing the study that was produced by The Howard Center for Family, Religion, & Society. This group is located in Rockford, Illinois and does not seem to be associated with Howard University. This group advocates for many conservative family values causes, and opposes gay marriage and no fault divorce. They regularly summarize research studies that support their political viewpoint. There are many other organizations, of course, that advocate viewpoints of all types and they also summarize studies that favor their political outlook.
There was a large discrepancy, however, in the information provided in the abstract and the information provided by this website. In particular, the website lists actual numbers from the publication itself while the abstract did not report any quantitative results.
Sharp differences in mortality rates separated these five groups, with men living alone, apart from their children, at greatest risk of premature death. In comparison with men living with a wife (or partner) and their children, fathers living alone—without spouse (or partner) and apart from their children—experienced “almost 4 times as great a risk of all-cause mortality, 10 times of death from external violence, 13 times from fall and poisoning, almost 5 times from suicide, and 19 times from addiction.”
You can read the full summary which does mention the attenuation of these effects after statistical adjustments, but does not report the adjusted rates, only the unadjusted ratios.
Questions
1. What do you think the "proxy variables to control for health-selection effects and socioeconomic circumstances" are?
2. Why is it important to adjust for these variables and why are proxies needed?
3. List some covariates which could possibly be imbalanced between the five groups studied other than health selection effects, socioeconomic circumstances, and presumably age. Could any of these influence mortality results?
4. Why would a reviewer be interested in the unadjusted ratios rather than the adjusted ratios?
5. Do you feel that the Howard Center for Family, Religion, & Society summary is a fair and balanced representation of the work by Ringback Weitoft et al? Be sure to read the full summary rather than my extract, because my summary of their summary could be biased.
6. Should the original authors have included more quantitative results in their abstract?
Submitted by Steve Simon